Short-term sensorimotor-based intervention for handwriting performance in elementary school children By: Adel A Alhusaini, Ganeswara Rao Melam and Syamala Buragadda

Short-term sensorimotor-based intervention for handwriting performance in elementary school children By: Adel A Alhusaini, Ganeswara Rao Melam and Syamala Buragadda

Abstract 

 Background: Handwriting problems in childhood can have lifelong repercussions, affecting learning and career. Sensorimotor intervention therefore helps to alleviate these problems.
 Methods: Thirty-one students (16 boys, 15 girls) underwent Minnesota Handwriting Assessment (MHA) to assess legibility, form, alignment, size, and spacing (the primary variables in this subject) as well as rate. Finally, 10 students (seven boys, three girls) aged 6–8 years (mean age, 77.1 +/- 1.45 months) participated in an intervention program. Baseline MHA, Handwriting Proficiency Screening Questionnaire (HPSQ), and grip strength were measured. The same group of students acted as their own controls and were analyzed before the interventions and later after completion of the protocol.
 

Results: There was a significant improvement in MHA scores for legibility, form, alignment, size and spacing (P < 0.05), with the exception of rate. There were also significant changes in legibility, time performance and physical and emotional wellbeing domains in the HPSQ, and grip strength (P < 0.05, paired t-test).
 Conclusion: Short-term sensorimotor-based intervention produced significant improvements in the handwriting performance of elementary school children.

 

 

 Handwriting is an academic skill that allows individuals to express their thoughts and feelings and communicate with others. The skill of handwriting is also referred to as a
 “graphomotor” skill, including visual–perceptual, orthographic coding, motor planning and execution, kinesthetic feedback and visual–motor coordination.1 Children spend 31–60% of their classroom time performing handwriting and other fine motor tasks, and difficulty in handwriting can influence academic achievement, which in turn affects self-esteem. (2–4) Although completing assignments by hand remains a prevalent practice in many elementary schools, an estimated 25–33% of students are struggling to achieve competency in this skill. (5,6) A total of 12–30% of children fail in the motor learning of
 handwriting and therefore are considered as poor writers or as having dysgraphia, (7) namely a learning disability that concerns the mechanical handwriting skill, unrelated to reading or spelling abilities. (8) Parents and teachers may not be aware of their child’s handwriting problem, which may have lifelong repercussions for the child and affect learning and career. Richards et al. note that handwriting is a “brain-based skill that facilitates meaning-making as writers externalize their cognitions through letter forms, the building blocks of written words and text”, further supporting the concept that handwriting is not merely a mechanical or motor skill. (9) Similarly, Christensen suggests that handwriting is not only about training the hand (motor skill); but it is also about how memory and orthographic processes work together to remember the letterforms and translate these figures onto the page automatically. (10)

The traditional art of letter writing is declining as rising numbers of children turn to email, text messages and social networking sites. It is frequently alleged that the pen is mightier than the sword, but a recent survey found that less of the student population writes every day. Psychologists and neuroscientists say it is far too soon to declare handwriting a relic of yesteryear. New evidence suggests that the links between
 handwriting and broader educational development run deep. (11,12)

 Handwriting/fine-motor difficulties are well documented as serious impediments to academic learning for kindergarten and elementary school children. Research suggests that handwriting influences reading, writing, (13–15) language and critical thinking. (16) Children with writing disorders have a tendency towards lower mathematics achievement, low verbal IQ, and increased attention difficulties, and consistently achieve lower marks compared with controls. (2,17–19) Handwriting difficulties in children include illegibility and inefficient writing speed. Legibility and performance time are important outcome parameters to assess and define poor handwriting. (20,21) In addition to legibility and timing deficits, children with dysgraphia complain more about fatigue and hand pain and are unwilling to write and do their homework; these signs represent a category of physical and emotional wellbeing. (21–24) Therefore, a child’s ability to write in a manner that is both legible and efficient, directly affects his or her school performance and academic advancement. Hence, it reinforces the importance of
 identifying handwriting difficulties as early as possible, both as a preventive and as a corrective aid.

 

 

 It is common for elementary-school children with handwriting difficulties including legibility problems to receive remedial education from their teacher or be referred to
 occupational therapy for evaluation and intervention. (6) The teacher is an important source of information about a child’s handwriting. Thus, teachers have been adopted as the main source of information to identify the non-proficient handwriters. (25–27) Communication with teachers is a decisive element for carry-over of intervention strategies to facilitate student success. (28) The common assumption among therapists is that a causal relationship exists between these lower-level processes and handwriting performance, and that remediation of these skills will result in improved handwriting. (29) Therefore, occupational therapists (OT) typically classify handwriting difficulties into sensorimotor (SM) and perceptual components in order to identify the foundation skills associated with the functional skill of handwriting. There is empirical evidence
 regarding the association of performance skills with handwriting, but the contribution of these performance skills is not well understood. (28–33) SM approaches use some combination of sensory input and motor activities to facilitate the expected normal motor response and promote motor skill development. (34–37)

 

 Research in the 20th century has led to several important advancements in the area of evaluation of handwriting difficulties, which are categorized as either global–holistic or as analytic evaluations. (20,38) The Handwriting Proficiency Screening Questionnaire (HPSQ) was designed for use by teachers or therapists to pinpoint handwriting deficiency among school age children. (21,39) The Minnesota Handwriting Test was developed to meet the need of OT for an educationally relevant, norm-referenced test that can identify students with handwriting difficulties as well as document treatment effectiveness. (40) Effectiveness of remedial handwriting intervention programs
 conducted by OT with positive outcomes were documented, (28) (35) but not much literature is available in the Saudi Arabian context.

 The present study will help to identify students in school who have poor handwriting and therefore help to improve overall academic performance. The purpose of this study was to evaluate the additive effects of SM intervention on the child’s handwriting. Research questions involved the possible effects of the intervention on specific qualities of handwriting (legibility, form, alignment, size, and spacing) and on speed. Secondary research questions involved possible effects of training in teacher assessment using the HPSQ and grip strength.

 Methods

Research design

 This prospective, one-group, pretest–post-test experimental design was used to evaluate the effectiveness of an SM intervention. We used a convenience sample of public school classes at the British International School, Riyadh, Saudi Arabia. The research ethics committee of the College of Applied Medical Sciences, King Saud University (CAMS 35–34/35 Ethics Number) and the School Authorities approved the
 study. Handwriting assessment was carried out before and after intervention.

 

Sample

 Students in two first grade classes for whom English was their first language, but who had no identified educational need, were selected to participate in the study. The parents were informed about the study during student orientation and completed consent forms at the time of the teachers’ initial assessment. Thirty-one students (16 boys, 15 girls) were screened using the Minnesota Handwriting Assessment (MHA), which assesses legibility, form, alignment, size, and spacing (the main variables in this study) as well as rate. Twenty students fulfilled the inclusion criteria for performing somewhat below peers or performing well below peers in at least two components of the MHA. Among them, 13 students agreed to participate in the intervention program, later two students left the school, and one student left the study before the actual intervention. Finally, 10 students (seven boys, three girls) aged 6–8 years (mean age, 77.1 +/- 1.45 months) participated in intervention program (Table 1). The research plan called for exclusion from the study for any student with direct intervention for handwriting problems prior to the study, and who missed more than two intervention sessions. The schoolteacher administered the HPSQ prior to initiation of the protocol. Handgrip strength was measured using a Jamar hand-held dynamometer (Jamar, Bolingbrook, IL, USA).

 

 

Instruments

The MHA was used in the current study as a screening tool and to measure outcome. (37,40) The near-point copy assessment analyzes Rate, Legibility, Form, Alignment, Size, and Spacing. Performance is then judged and classified as performing like peers (upper 75th percentile); performing below peers (bottom 5–25th percentile); and performing well-below their peers (bottom 5th percentile).  Test sheets for the MHA are available in both standard manuscript and D’Nealian script style to fit the instructional style of each class. Students copy words from a pre-printed example onto marked lines below the example. The example (“the quick brown fox jumped over the lazy dogs”) includes all letters of the alphabet. The handwriting sample was scored based on specific directions for each category (legibility, form, alignment, size, and spacing) and rate. Each category was scored based on the error rate with a maximum potential score of 34. Rate or speed was scored by counting the number of
 letters completed in 2.5 min. (37,40) The HPSQ was used to identify handwriting deficiency among school-aged children and is appropriate for varied academic and clinical purposes. The tool has good internal consistency (a = 0.90). Score test–retest reliability had an intra class correlation coefficient (ICC) of 0.84 and ICC interrater reliability 0.92. (21,39) Non-proficient handwriters were classified as having HPSQ score ≥14, and proficient handwriters as having HPSQ score <14.  A calibrated hydraulic hand-held dynamometer, the Jamar hand-held dynamometer, was used to measure isometric grip strength for both the dominant and non-dominant hand. (41,42)

 

Table 1 Subject characteristics

Characteristic  Mean +/- SD or n
Age months (n = 10) 77.1 +/- 1.45
Hand dominance Right/Left 8/2
Gender Male/Female 7/3
Ethnicity (Arab/British) 8/2

 

Table 2 MHA scores for within-subjects factors (Manuscript)

  Manuscript
 (n=10)
Time MHA  score
 (Mean  +/- SD)
F P-value
Rate Baseline 20 +/- 7.77 3.886

0.080

(n.s.)


Before Intervention 21.3 +/- 3.31


After Intervention 27.4 +/- 7.94

Legibility Baseline 21 +/- 8.52 22.965 0.001*

Before Intervention 26.5 +/- 7.42


After Intervention 33.7 +/- 0.68

Form Baseline 17.50 +/- 5.29 61.682 0.000*

Before Intervention 20.80 +/- 5.54


After Intervention

30.80 +/- 2.15



Alignment Baseline 5.80 +/- 6.85 72.610 0.000*

Before Intervention 18.00 +/- 4.85


After Intervention 30.90 +/- 4.31

Size Baseline 8.20 +/- 8.35 66.122 0.000*

Before Intervention 20.20 +/- 4.49


After Intervention 32.20 +/- 2.74

Spacing Baseline 20.50 +/- 8.18 13.183 0.005*

Before Intervention 23.00 +/- 5.85


After Intervention 30.90 +/- 3.57

 *P ≤ 0. 05 (repeated-measures ANOVA). MHA, Minnesota Handwriting Assessment; ns, nonsignificant.

 

Table 3 MHA scores for within-subjects factors (D’Nealian)

 Manuscript
 (n=10)
Time MHA score
 (Mean +/- SD)
F  P-Value
Rate Baseline 20.60 +/- 6.68 31.954 0.000*

Before Intervention 25.0 +/- 7.71


After Intervention 33.6 +/- 1.27

Legibility Baseline 26.7 +/- 5.58 15.281 0.004*

Before Intervention 27.0 +/- 6.18


After Intervention 33.8 +/- 0.42

Form Baseline 22.00 +/- 5.01 25.175 0.001*

Before Intervention 23.20 +/- 4.98


After Intervention 30.60 +/- 1.65

Alignment Baseline 8.60 +/- 7.98 58.827 0.000*

Before Intervention 20.00 +/- 8.69


After Intervention 29.80 +/- 4.94

Size Baseline 11.30 +/- 8.76 57.168 0.000*

Before Intervention 22.20 +/- 9.17


After Intervention 32.40 +/- 1.51

Spacing Baseline 25.50 +/- 6.67 8.288 0.018*

Before Intervention 25.70 +/- 5.36


After Intervention 31.00 +/- 1.76

*P ≤ 0. 05 (repeated-measures ANOVA). MHA, Minnesota Handwriting Assessment.

 

 

Table 4 Post-intervention changes in MHA score for Manuscript and D’Nealian style

Manuscript (n=10) t  P-value 
Rate -2.141 0.061 (n.s.)
Legibility -2.951 0.016*
Form -5.752 0.000*
Alignment -8.310 0.000*
Size -10.757 0.000*
Spacing -4.570 0.001*
D’Nealian (n=10)

Rate -3.459 0.007 (n.s.)
Legibility -3.374 0.008*
Form -3.805 0.004*
Alignment -4.889 0.001*
Size -10.757 0.000*
Spacing -2.994 0.015*

 *P≤0. 05 (paired-sample t-test). MHA, Minnesota Handwriting Assessment; ns, nonsignificant.

 

Table 5 Change in HPSQ score and grip strength

 Characteristics  Before
 (Mean +/- SD)
After
 (Mean +/- SD)

 P-value
HPSQ


Legibility domain 5.80 +/- 3.58 2.90 +/- 2.52 0.020*
Time Performance 6.00 +/- 3.24 4.00 +/- 2.49 0.019*
Physical and emotional wellbeing 8.50 +/- 6.02 3.10 +/- 2.81 0.012*

Total

19.90 +/- 9.66 10.00 +/- 6.47  0.002*
Grip Strength


DOM grip 17.50 +/- 5.89 20.50 +/- 5.51 0.024*
NDOM grip 14.00 +/- 6.99 18.00 +/- 4.22 0.022*

*P ≤ 0. 05 (paired-sample t-test). DOM, dominant; HPSQ, Handwriting Proficiency Screening Questionnaire; NDOM, non-dominant.

 

 

Procedure

This survey was conducted during March2014–March 2015. The children (n=31; 16 boys, 15 girls) from two first grade classes completed the MHA. Students are seated at the appropriate height for their size in a distraction-free environment. A stimulus sheet for both the manuscript and D’Nealian-style handwriting were given to each child after explaining the directions of the writing process as per the kit guidelines. After evaluating MHA scores, the students who fulfilled the inclusion criteria were chosen to take part in the study (n = 20). Parents were informed of the voluntary participation of
 the students in the sensory intervention program to improve their handwriting skills, by the school authorities. Ultimately, 10 students (seven boys, three girls) agreed to participate in the study. These students were promoted to second grade at the start of the intervention. Baseline HPSQ, MHA and grip strength were measured. The same group of students acted as their own controls, and were assessed before intervention and later after completion of the protocol.

 The SM-based intervention group met twice per week (Monday and Wednesday) from 11:45 am to 12:35 pm for 5 weeks during regularly scheduled school hours. These students were given training in separate classrooms. Each 40 min session consisted of 10 min of gross and fine motor warm-up activities,43 followed by 30 min of SM component activities in visual perception, visual–motor integration, proprioception/  kinesthesia, and in-hand manipulation. (29) The intervention was provided in two small groups in quiet, private rooms at the schools. One OT and a physical therapist with pediatric experience along with a special education teacher were present during the intervention. Therapists were supplied with a toolbox of games, activity worksheets, equipment, and treatment ideas for each factor, as well as the schedule for rotation of activities. (29) All the students participated in the initial warm-up session and requested to do all SM activities in random order. The treating therapists and students, however, were permitted flexibility in choosing the activities during each session from the list supplied, and to vary the format, order, and the materials used in the interventions. Therapists and teachers assigned to the stations monitor students and provide specific feedback, support and instruction as needed. The students rotated through the stations in random order to perform the list of activities (Appendix S1) and therapists emphasized performance of all the activities. 

 

Statistical analysis

Data were analyzed using SPSS 22 (IBM Corp, (2013), Armonk, NY, USA) and 95% CI. Descriptive statistics were used for demographic characteristics. The level of significance
 was set at 0.05. Repeated-measures ANOVA was used for within-group comparisons. Pairwise comparisons were analyzed using independent T-test.

 Results

 Thirty-one children participated in the study, and 10 students (seven male, three female) who met the inclusion criteria completed the whole study. The subjects acted as their controls at baseline and followed without intervention for 6 months, and later were reassessed before and after a short-term SM intervention for a period of 5 weeks. The features of the sample  are reported in Table 1.Within subjects factors for Manuscript
MHA and D’Nealian-MHA are reported in tables 2 and 3. Post intervention changes in MHA score for Manuscript & D’Nealian style are reported in table 4. Paired-sample t-test (HPSQ) and Grip strength are reported in table 5. 

Discussion

 This study addressed the need for further research on handwriting remediation and we investigated the effectiveness of short-term SM intervention on handwriting performance in elementary school children. The first hypothesis was that children receiving intervention would show substantial improvement in handwriting performance. The short-term training had beneficial effects on MHA scores of rate, legibility, form, alignment, size and spacing in both the manuscript and D’Nealian scripts. The teacher perception of student handwriting assessed on HPSQ showed a significant difference in all the domains, that is, legibility, performance time, physical and emotional components after the intervention. Handgrip strength improved after the intervention, as recorded with Jamar hand-held dynamometer. 

The students were followed longitudinally from grade 1 without any intervention until grade 2, thereafter the short term SM intervention program was carried out for 5 weeks,
 twice per week. The short intervention produced a remarkable improvement in handwriting MHA scores, except for rate in manuscript style, but showed improvement in D’Nealian script. The rate scores were not significantly improved, which was supported by other studies, given that rate is not associated with function. Studies examining the correlation between handwriting speed and legibility have yielded contradictory findings. (37) Given that students are instructed to write as neatly as possible, the speed of handwriting is decreased despite the intervention. The improvement in D’Nealian script rate score may be attributed to the curriculum of the school. The other variables of MHA, such as legibility, form, alignment, size and spacing reached statistical significance (P < 0.05). The strong individual scores for improvement in these variables is attributed to the SM intervention. The program has components similar to the handwriting and writing interventions developed by Denton et al. emphasizing critical elements of SM intervention. (29,32) Ninety percent of Canadian OT and 92% of US school-based OT surveyed used SM approaches to remediate handwriting. (31,33)

Earlier studies have been conducted to explore potential factors that may contribute to handwriting difficulties. The intrinsic factors include kinesthesia, fine motor skills, eye–hand coordination, visual perceptual skills, and visual motor integration, and extrinsic factors include writing posture, chair, and desk height; these are believed to have a relationship with handwriting. (2) In this study, emphasis was therefore placed on both intrinsic and extrinsic factors to improve handwriting skills. In the SM approach, interventions are oriented to the child’s sensory systems, and provide a variety of sensory experiences with appropriate feedback to improve motor performance. Apart from the SM approach, additional factors such as paper position, writing surface, grip strength, and posture were also considered.

 

 

Therapists acted in close collaboration with teachers to eliminate these extrinsic factors and thereafter focus on motor planning abilities. Case-Smith et al. developed an integrated handwriting program that produced
 large, significant gains in handwriting legibility and speed, which were maintained at 6 month follow up. (28,34) Mackay et al. found that the Log Handwriting Program improved children’s writing legibility. Legibility improved by a mean of 4.1
 points (95% CI : 2.5–5.7); form, 5.3 points; alignment, 7.8 points; size, 7.9 points; and space, 5.3 points. Speed decreased by 3.9 points.33 The present results agree with these aforementioned studies, emphasizing the importance of SM-based intervention taking into account the extrinsic factors. Other SM based programs have resulted in little or no effect on handwriting performance when compared with traditional handwriting instruction. (29,30,37)

Limitations

Pupils in this study were chosen via convenience sample from the British International School in Riyadh. The initial plan was to recruit students for control and experimental groups, but this could not be done due to low recruitment numbers.  The sample size was small and limited to parents who agreed to have their child involved in the study. Whether a single intervention or a combination of intervention strategies was the cause of improvement is not known. This could have biased the sample, because there was no control group in the study. Students in this study were homogeneous, having a similar preschool experience.

 

Future research

 The authors recommend that the subjective and objective standards of handwriting analysis must be implemented in schools to analyze handwriting difficulties. Thus, sensitive and evidence-based strategies, rather than intuition-based strategies, need to be used to right the problems in hand. While teachers are skilled at informally identifying poor handwriting, the MHT and HPSQ help to support the teacher’s subjective judgement of poor-quality writing. This evidence-based approach helps to document the problem and plan adequate measures to counter the problem in the early phases. The role of handwriting efficiency in the writing of young children has been underestimated in mainstream education and appropriate SM interventions are helpful. SM intervention is an evidence-based protocol that can be recommended by practitioners for effective printing instruction in the classroom or institutional level.

Acknowledgments

 The authors extend their appreciation to the Deanship of Scientific Research at King Saud University for funding this work through research group no. RGP-256. We are indebted to the children and their parents of the British International School, Riyadh, for enthusiastic participation, and extend special thanks to school teachers involved in the present study. We also thank Mr. Emad Takrouni for assistance in collecting the data.

Disclosure

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

 

Author contributions

A.A.A. and G.R.M. designed the study; G.R.M. and S.B. collected data; A.A.A. and S.B. analyzed the data; A.A.A., G.R.M., and S.B. wrote the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

References

 1 Volman MJ, van Schendel BM, Jongmans MJ. Handwriting difficulties in primary school children: A search for underlying mechanisms. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 2006; 60: 451-60.
 2 Feder KP, Majnemer A. Handwriting development, competency and intervention Review. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2007; 49: 312–7.
 3 McHale K, Cermak SA. Fine motor activities in elementary school: Preliminary findings and provisional implications for children with fine motor problems. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 1992; 46: 898–903.
 4 Smith-Zuzovsky N, Exner CE. The effect of seated positioning quality on typical 6- and 7-year-old children’s object manipulation skills. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 2004; 58: 380–8.
 5 Graham S, and Santangelo T. “A Meta-Analysis of the Effectiveness of Teaching Handwriting”. Presented at Handwriting in the 21st Century?: An Educational Summit,
 Washington, D.C., January 23, 2012.
 6 Marr D, Dimeo SB. Outcomes associated with a summer handwriting course for elementary students. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 2006; 60:10–5.
 7 Nicolson RI, Fawcett AJ. Dyslexia, dysgraphia, procedural learning and the cerebellum. Cortex 2011; 47: 117–27.
 8 Danna J, Velay JL. Basic and supplementary sensory feedback in handwriting. Front. Psychol. 2015; 6: 169.
 9 Richards TL, Berninger VW, Stock P et al. Differences between good and poor child writers on fMRI contrasts for writing newly taught and highly practiced letterforms. Read. Writ. 2011; 24: 493–516.
 10 Christensen CA. The role of orthographic-motor integration in the production of creative and well-structured written text for students in secondary school. Educ. Psychol. 2005; 25:441–53.
 11 Medwell J, Wray D. Handwriting: A forgotten language skill? Lang. Educ. 2008; 22:34–47.
 12 Berninger VW, Richard T, Stock P et al. fMRI activation related to nature of ideas generated and differences between good and poor writers during idea generation. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. Psychol. Monogr. Series II 2009; 6:77–93.
 13 Fitzgerald J, Shanahan T. Reading and writing relations and their development. Educ. Psychol. 2000; 35:39–50.
 14 Edwards L. Writing instruction in kindergarten: Examining an emerging area of research for children with writing and reading difficulties. J. Learn. Disabil. 2003; 36: 136–48.

 15 Bourdin B, Fayol M. Is graphic activity cognitively costly? A developmental approach. Read Writ. 2000; 13 (3–4): 183–96.
 16 Saperstein Associates. Handwriting in the 21st Century? Research Shows Why Handwriting Belongs in Today’s Classroom: A Summary of Research Presented at Handwriting in the 21st Century? An Educational Summit. 2012. [Cited 2015 December 12.] Available from URL: http://www.sapersteinassociates.com/wpcontent/uploads/2015/11/2_29_HW_Summit_White_Paper_eVersion.pdf
 17 Lowis MJ, Mooney S. Examination performance and graphological analysis of students’ handwriting. Percept. Mot. Skills 2001; 93: 367–81.
 18 Sandler AD, Watson TE, Footo M, Levine MD, Coleman WL, Hooper SR. Neurodevelopmental study of writing disorders in middle childhood. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 1992; 13:17–23.
 19 Vander Hart N, Fitzpatrick P, Cortesa C. In-depth analysis of handwriting curriculum and instruction in four kindergarten classrooms. Read. Writ. 2010; 23: 673–99.
 20 Rosenblum S, Weiss PL, Parush S. Product and process evaluation of handwriting difficulties. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2003; 15:41–81.
 21 Rosenblum S. Development, reliability, and validity of the Handwriting Proficiency Screening Questionnaire (HPSQ).  Am. J. Occup. Ther. 2008; 62: 298–307.
 22 Tseng MH, Cermak SA. The influence of ergonomic factors and perceptual– motor abilities on handwriting performance. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 1993; 47: 919–26.
 23 Engel-Yeger B, Nagauker-Yanuv L, Rosenblum S. Handwriting performance, self-reports, and perceived self-efficacy among children with dysgraphia. Am. J. Occup. Ther.
 2009; 63: 182–92.
 24 Rosenblum S, Aloni T, Josman N. Relationships between handwriting performance and organizational abilities among children with and without dysgraphia: A preliminary study. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2010; 31: 502–9.
 25 Asher AV. Handwriting instruction in elementary schools. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 2006; 60: 461–71.
 26 Hammerschmidt SL, Sudsawad P. Teachers’ survey on problems with handwriting: Referral, evaluation, and outcomes. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 2004; 58: 185–92.
 27 Sudsawad P, Trombly CA, Henderson A, Tickle-Degnen L. The relationship between the Evaluation Tool of Children’s Handwriting and teachers’ perceptions of handwriting
 legibility. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 2001; 55: 518–23.
 28 Case-Smith J. Effectiveness of school-based occupational therapy intervention on handwriting. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 2002; 56:17–25.
 29 Denton PL, Cope S, Moser C. The effects of sensorimotor based intervention versus therapeutic practice on improving handwriting performance in 6- to 11-year-old children. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 2006; 60:16–27.
 30 Zwicker J, Hadwin A. Cognitive versus multisensory approaches to handwriting intervention: A randomized controlled trial. OTJR 2009; 29:40–8.
 31 Woodward S, Swinth Y. Multisensory approach to handwriting remediation: Perceptions of school-based  occupational therapists. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 2002; 56:
 305–12.
 32 Case-Smith J, Holland T, Lane A, White S. Effect of a coteaching handwriting program for first graders: One-group pretest-posttest design. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 2012; 66: 396 405.
 33 Mackay N, McCluskey A, Mayes R. The Log Handwriting Program improved children’s writing legibility: A pretest-posttest study. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 2010; 64:30–6.
 34 Case-Smith J, Holland T, Bishop B. Effectiveness of an integrated handwriting program for first grade students: A pilot study. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 2011; 65: 670–8.
 35 Weintraub N, Yinon M, Hirsch IBE, Parush S. Effectiveness of sensorimotor and task-oriented handwriting intervention in elementary school-aged students with handwriting difficulties. OTJR 2009; 29: 125–34.
 36 Howe TH, Roston KL, Sheu CF, Hinojosa J. Assessing handwriting intervention effectiveness in elementary school students: A two-group controlled study. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 2013; 67:19–26.
 37 Peterson CQ, Nelson DL. Effect of an occupational intervention on printing in children with economic disadvantages. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 2005; 7: 152–60.
 38 Feder KP, Majnemer A. Children’s handwriting evaluation tools and their psychometric properties. Phys. Occup. Ther. Pediatr. 2003; 23 (3): 65–84. 

39 Rosenblum S, Gafni-Lachter L. Handwriting Proficiency Screening Questionnaire for Children (HPSQ-C): Development, reliability, and validity. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 2015; 69 (3): 1–9. 

40 Reisman JE. Development and reliability of the research version of the Minnesota Handwriting Test. Phys. Occup. Ther. Pediatr. 1993; 13 (2): 41–55. 

41 van den Beld, Willeke A, van der Sanden GA, Sengers RC, Verbeek AL, Gabreels FJ. Validity and reproducibility of the Jamar dynamometer in children aged 4-11 years. Disabil. Rehabil. 2006; 28: 1303–9.
 42 Holm I, Fredriksen P, Fosdahl M, Vollestad N. A normative sample of isotonic and isokinetic muscle strength measurements in children 7 to 12 years of age. Acta Paediatr.
 2008; 97: 602–7.
 43 Keller M. Handwriting club: Using sensory integration strategies to improve handwriting. Interv. Sch. Clinic 2001; 31 (1): 9–12.

 

Supporting Information

Additional Supporting Information may be found in the online version of this article at the publisher’s web-site.

Appendix S1 Sensorimotor activities for elementary school children

Back to blog